Pet Ownership, but Not ACE Inhibitor Therapy, Blunts Home Blood Pressure Responses to Mental Stress
Karen Allen, Barbara E. Shykoff, Joseph L. Izzo, Jr
Abstract—In the present study, we evaluated the effect of a nonevaluative social support intervention (pet ownership) on
blood pressure response to mental stress before and during ACE inhibitor therapy. Forty-eight hypertensive individuals participated in an experiment at home and in the physician’s office. Participants were randomized to an experimental group with assignment of pet ownership in addition to lisinopril (20 mg/d) or to a control group with only lisinopril (20 mg/d). On each study day, blood pressure, heart rate, and plasma renin activity were recorded at baseline and after each mental stressor (serial subtraction and speech). Before drug therapy, mean responses to mental stress did not differ significantly between experimental and control groups in heart rate (94 [SD 6.8] versus 93 [6.8] bpm), systolic blood pressure (182 [8.0] versus 181 [8.3] mm Hg), diastolic blood pressure (120 [6.6] versus 119 [7.9] mm Hg), or plasma renin activity (9.4 [0.59] versus 9.3 [0.57] ng · mLϪ1 · hϪ1). Lisinopril therapy lowered resting blood pressure by Ϸ35/20 mm Hg in both groups, but responses to mental stress were significantly lower among pet owners relative to those who only received lisinopril (PϽ0.0001; heart rate 81 [6.3] versus 91 [6.5] bpm, systolic blood pressure 131 [6.8] versus 141 [7.8] mm Hg, diastolic blood pressure 92 [6.3] versus 100 [6.8] mm Hg, and plasma renin activity 13.9 [0.92] versus 16.1 [0.58] ng · mLϪ1 · hϪ1). We conclude that ACE inhibitor therapy alone lowers resting blood pressure, whereas increased social support through pet ownership lowers blood pressure response to mental stress. (Hypertension. 2001;38:815-820.) Key Words: blood pressure Ⅲ social support Ⅲ stress Ⅲ lifestyle Ⅲ pets
Antihypertensive agents are known to reliably lower uals with pets are buffered from the impact of stressful life
resting blood pressure, but most of these drugs have
events and make fewer visits to physicians14; among persons
little effect on blood pressure responses to physical or mental
with AIDS, pet owners have a lower incidence of depression
stressors.1,2 Because individuals who experience pronounced,
than do those without pets.15 Finally, service dogs have a
frequent, or enduring autonomically mediated cardiovascular
positive influence on the well-being, self-esteem, and com-
responses to stress may be at risk for the development of
munity integration of persons with disabilities.16
cardiovascular disease,3,4 variables that moderate or mediate
In the present study, we extended previous laboratory
reactivity to stress are important to consider. Several
stress-reactivity and social support research to include home
laboratory- and community-based studies have focused on the
data in randomly assigned pet owners administered the ACE
potential role of social support in buffering reactivity to
inhibitor lisinopril. We hypothesized that the acquisition of a
mental stress5–9 and have found that when social support
pet would reduce heart rate, blood pressure, and renin
participants are perceived as supportive and nonevaluative, a
responses to psychological stress among a group of hyper-
tensive individuals in a high-stress profession (stockbrokers).
Although considerable attention has been devoted to the
The design we used made it possible to demonstrate the
definition and measurement of social support as it relates to
independence of blood pressure reactivity from basal blood
health,10,11 most of this literature assumes that benefits are
provided only by humans. In recent years, however, severalstudies have documented that pet animals also can have an
important supportive role and a positive influence on thehealth of their owners. Pet ownership is a significant predictor
Participants and Setting
of 1-year survival after myocardial infarction.12,13 Relative to
Participants were hypertensive patients with a high-stress occupa-tion. A pretest-posttest control group design was used17 in 48
the support of friends and spouses, the presence of a pet
volunteers who had uncomplicated stage IIϩ hypertension (resting
elicits significantly lower blood pressure and heart rate
blood pressure Ն160/100 mm Hg). Of the 24 men (18 white and 6
reactivity during mental stress.5 In addition, elderly individ-
black) and 24 women (18 white and 6 black), all were interested in
Received December 26, 2000; first decision February 23, 2001; revision accepted March 26, 2001. From the Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Department of Medicine, State University of New York at Buffalo. Correspondence to Karen Allen, PhD, Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Department of Medicine, State University of New York at Buffalo, Millard
Fillmore Hospital, 3 Gates Circle, Buffalo, NY 14209. E-mail [email protected]
2001 American Heart Association, Inc. Hypertension is available at http://www.hypertensionaha.org 815 816 Hypertension October 2001 Six-Month Results for Lisinopril and Pet Ownership: F Values for Main Effects and Interactions With SBP, DBP, HR, and PRA
stress reduction and agreed to acquire a pet if chosen to do so.
Six months later, the data collection procedure just described was
Participants were randomized to a control group without pets (nϭ24)
repeated (in both the physician’s office and in the homes of
or an experimental group (nϭ24) who subsequently acquired pets.
participants). Pet owners performed the second phase of the homeexperiment in the presence of their pets, which roamed freely
throughout the room in which data collection took place.
All participants completed baseline mental stress sessions in theirhomes after 1 month of observation. All participants then were
Data Analysis
treated with lisinopril (20 mg/d). Those assigned to the pet owner
The main analysis was a repeated measures ANOVA before and
group acquired their animals at the time drug therapy began. All
during drug therapy, with tasks (MAT and speech) as within-subjects
participants were evaluated again at 6 months with a second home
factors and pet ownership status and score on self-report social
mental stress session. Dependent measures were systolic blood
support scale (categorized as high or low) as between-subjects
pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), heart rate (HR), and
factors. All analyses were performed separately for SBP, DBP, HR,
plasma renin activity (PRA). Participant ratings of stress and coping
and PRA. Additional repeated measures ANOVAs addressed home
were assessed both before and after each stressor.
versus office blood pressure values and a comparison of MATperformance before and with drug therapy and pet ownership. Stressors
An expanded Methods section can be found in an online data
Stressors included mental arithmetic task (MAT) and speech, both of
supplement available at http://www.hypertensionaha.org.
which have been used in numerous laboratories18 to produce sub-stantial increases in cardiovascular response. Physiological Recording Instrument Physiological Responses: Experimental Findings
HR, SBP, and DBP were recorded automatically once each minute
ANOVAs for SBP, DBP, and HR revealed that before
throughout the experiment with a portable Propaq monitor (model
individuals began drug therapy or acquired pets, there were
main effects (PϽ0.01) for social support and for tasks (MATand speech), as well as for social supportϫtask interactions,
Procedures
but no effects by assigned pet ownership status. Before drug
After participants provided written informed consent, they wereseated in a quiet room, and the Propaq blood pressure cuff was
therapy and pets, ANOVA results for PRA also revealed main
attached. Resting HR, SBP, DBP, and PRA then were assessed and
effects (PϽ0.01) for MAT and speech but not for social
recorded. In addition, participants completed a questionnaire about
social support.19 Later in the same day, participants performed 2
The Table includes SBP, DBP, HR, and PRA ANOVA
psychologically stressful tasks in their homes according to a standard
summary data after 6 months with lisinopril and pets; except
laboratory paradigm. HR, SBP, and DBP were recorded once eachminute throughout the experiment. Renin was assessed 3 times (ie,
for DBP during MAT, main effects are shown for pet
after the initial rest and after each of the stressful tasks).
ownership condition as well as social support and tasks for
After this home experiment, all participants in both the experi-
SBP and DBP. Results for HR and PRA are similar but
mental and control groups began lisinopril therapy (20 mg/d) with
include no main effects for social support. In addition, the
the goal of blood pressure within a normal range (Ͻ130/90 mm Hg).
Table shows significant 2-way interactions (between pet
Black participants also received 12.5 mg/d hydrochlorothiazide. Atthe time they began drug therapy, individuals in the experimental
condition and tasks and between social support and tasks) for
group were instructed to acquire a pet cat or dog.
all dependent variables, as well as 3-way interactions among
Allen et al Pets Blunt Reactivity 817 Figure 1. SBP and DBP reactivity (mean [SEM]) in response to MAT and speech both before and during lisinopil therapy and the pres- ence of a pet.
pet condition, social support, and tasks for all dependent
ratios after participants received lisinopril and acquired pets.
Results revealed a main effect for ratio (Fϭ166.90, PϽ0.001)
Figure 1 provides SBP and DBP reactivity for each task,
and an interaction effect for lisinopril and pet ownership
and Figure 2 provides HR and PRA (both before and during
(Fϭ140, PϽ0.001.) That is, at the second data collection
drug therapy). At the beginning of the study, all participants
point (after the acquisition of pets), pet owners had signifi-
had stage II hypertension (Ͼ160/100 mm Hg), and after 6
cantly greater improvements in their task performances than
months, average blood pressures in both the lisinopril-only
and the lisinopril-and-pet groups were within the normalrange. At month 1, MAT and speech elicited significant
Discussion
increases in physiological responses of both groups. After 6
In the present study, we examined the effect of pet ownership
months, however, relative to those without pets, individuals
on cardiovascular responses to psychological stress among a
with pets had significantly lower reactivity scores; their
group of hypertensive individuals in high-stress professions.
On the basis of these results, we conclude that reactivity andbasal blood pressure are influenced by independent mecha-
Comparisons of Office With Home Blood Pressure
nisms; that is, ACE inhibitor therapy lowers only resting
Before participants received the medication and acquired
blood pressure, whereas the addition of a social support
pets, there was a significant difference between office and
intervention lowers responses to stress. These results extend
home mean SBP (Fϭ2089.36, PϽ0.001) and DBP
earlier findings that ACE inhibition fails to diminish BP or
(Fϭ4499.23, PϽ0.001) values. When individuals were ad-
HR responses to stressful tasks1 to demonstrate a beneficial
ministered medication and acquired pets, blood pressure was
influence of social factors that may buffer stress responses for
again higher in the office than in the home (SBP [Fϭ344.94,
persons with hypertension who are treated with ACE
PϽ0.001] and DBP [Fϭ1657.37, PϽ0.001]). There were no
other main effects associated with these differences.
Interestingly, we found that an individual’s assessment of
his or her general social environment was predictive of that
Performance Data
person’s BP and HR responses to stress. Even though we did
MAT performance was computed for each participant as a
not investigate the effect of the presence of friends on
ratio between the number of correct answers and the number
reactivity, participants who perceived that they belonged to a
of attempted answers. We were interested in a comparison of
group and had friends to confide in had a lower reactivity to
ratios before participants had received lisinopril and pets with
stress than did their counterparts who reported few social
818 Hypertension October 2001 Figure 2. HR and PRA reactivity (mean [SEM]) in response to MAT and speech both before and during lisinopril therapy and the pres- ence of a pet.
contacts. These results suggest that persons with low social
voluntary meeting, and all dog owners and most cat owners
support systems are likely to benefit in particular from the
brought their pets with them to the session. The consensus
enhanced environment that pets can provide.
response with the highest rating was: “Having this pet makes
Improved task performance was also associated with pet
me better able to see what is really important and to put things
ownership. At the beginning of the study, participants in both
into perspective.” When asked about increased responsibility
the control group (lisinopril only) and the experimental group
and similar issues, participants responded that the positive
(lisinopril and pet ownership) had 74% correct performance.
aspects far outweighed any added expense or responsibility
At the second data collection point, however, participants
and that they would never give up their pets. Because only
with pets had 92% correct performance, whereas their coun-
persons who would agree to acquire pets were eligible to
terparts without pets remained at 75%. This finding is notable
participate in our study, however, we cannot comment on
because improved task performance suggests that participants
whether individuals who are not inclined to like animals
did not abandon the task because they perceived their pets to
would develop similar relationships with pets.
be pleasant distractions. In addition, although ACE inhibitor
The issue of demand characteristics (expectations uninten-
therapy has been associated with either improved or impaired
tionally conveyed from the investigator to the participants) is
cognitive function,20 in our study lisinopril alone did not have
an important factor in behavioral research. Consequently, at
any influence on cognition, and cognitive improvement
the beginning of our project, we did not reveal our hypotheses
occurred only when lisinopril was paired with the presence of
about pets but rather said the focus of the study was on the
general relationship between social factors and health. In the
One explanation for our findings is that the presence of
debriefing session at the conclusion of the study, we asked
pets provided the kind of nonevaluative social support that is
participants to identify whether they thought pets influenced
critical to buffering physiological responses to stress. Social
their resting blood pressure, their responses to stress, or both.
support theorists10,19 have suggested that positive feeling
Except for 2 participants who answered “both,” all said that
states may enhance an individual’s capacity to adapt to stress.
they believed their pets helped diminish their resting blood
We believe that pets may evoke such feelings in their owners.
pressure. This was reinforced by the fact that participants
This conclusion was confirmed by an exit conference in
performed home monitoring over the course of the study and
which the nominal group technique21 was used to structure
that lisinopril therapy did in fact cause a dramatic reduction in
responses to the question, “How has your pet changed your
blood pressure. The pet owners, however, attributed this
life?” Interestingly, all of our participants attended this
reduction to a combination of drug therapy and pet owner-
Allen et al Pets Blunt Reactivity 819
ship. Because of this fortuitous misunderstanding on the part
we cannot generalize the findings to other stressful settings,
of our participants, we do not believe that demand character-
such as work environments. Because differences between
istics contributed to our findings about stress responses.
office and home resting blood pressures were not influenced
Because the participants (both with and without pets) were
by pet ownership or lisinopril therapy, it is logical to consider
highly motivated to reduce their blood pressure and believed
whether stress reactivity outside the home might also not be
that the drug would work, however, we cannot totally rule out
influenced by pets. We believe, however, that because reac-
that demand characteristics as well as a placebo effect
tivity and resting blood pressure are independent of each
contributed to their reductions in resting blood pressure. That
other, reactivity outside the home has the potential to be
is, wanting to please experimenters who came to their homes,
combined with a strong belief in the treatment, could have
In the present study, we were able to change the social
influenced responses to lisinopril, although research suggests
environment of our participants by adding a pet to their lives.
that actual placebos have little effect on ambulatory blood
Enhancement of social support with human friends is much
pressure monitoring,22 so we believe this was unlikely.
more complex and difficult to achieve and, to our knowledge,
We acknowledge that the study population sample was
has not been successfully carried out in an experimental
highly selected for homogeneity. We were interested in
design. Because pets, unlike human friends, are perceived as
looking at stress responses in a group of individuals who
always being nonjudgmental and accepting of their owners,
experienced similar job stress and who lived alone. Because
they are ideal candidates for social support intervention.
there were many stockbrokers available who were motivated
Physiologically, pets had greater influence on sympathetic
to be in a behavioral study and were willing to acquire pets,
responses than did ACE inhibition alone. Consequently, our
we decided to focus on them. Although chronic stress may be
findings suggest that higher center influences can modify
associated with an elevated renin level, we did not select
stress responses and that coping skills may be related to
participants on this basis or for any physiological character-
increased cortical inhibition of the brain stem. Although we
istic other than stage II hypertension. The baseline PRA that
do not advocate the substitution of pets for human compan-
we report may appear higher than is often observed, but it is
ionship, we conclude that for persons who like animals and
consistent with a previous related study.1 In addition, our
have few social contacts, pets can enhance isolated lives and
work supports earlier studies that document a positive rela-
tionship between psychological stress and renin reactivi-ty.23,24 In the present study, because the main interest was in
Acknowledgments
a change from baseline, the reported difference in change
This work was supported by Food and Drug Administration grant
scores between groups is the important area of focus. How-
FDT-000889 and by the Waltham Center for Pet Nutrition(Waltham-on-the-Wolds, England). We are very appreciative of
ever, because it is known that individuals with high renin
important contributions made to this study by the late Patrick
levels often have a very positive response to ACE inhibition
therapy,25 the possibility exists that our findings wouldgeneralize only to other “high-renin” individuals. Among our
References
participants, the degree of response to lisinopril was consis-
1. Dimsdale JE, Mills P, Ziegler M, Leitz K, Nelesen R. Converting enzyme
tent with renin response to stress before drug therapy. We can
inhibition and blood pressure reactivity to psychological stressors. Hyper-tension. 1992;20:210 –213.
only speculate whether elevated renin is another manifesta-
2. Mills P, Dimsdale JE. Cardiovascular reactivity to psychological
tion of generalized neural hormonal activation that likely
stressors: a review of the effects of beta-blockade. Psychosomatics. 1991;
includes increased sympathetic nervous system and adreno-
3. Clarkson TB, Manuck SB, Kaplan JR. Potential role of cardiovascular
reactivity in atherogenesis. In: Matthews KA, Weiss SM, Detre T, Dem-
The present study has several limitations. Although the
broski TM, Falkner B, Manuck SB, Williams RB, eds. Handbook of
study design could have been strengthened by the addition of
Stress, Reactivity, and Cardiovascular Disease. New York, NY: Wiley;
an “intervention” placebo group, we did not include one
4. Gullette ECD, Blumenthal JJ, Babyak M, Jiang W, Waugh RA, Frid DJ,
because of the nature of our social intervention. We do not
O’Connor CM, Morris JJ, Krantz DS. Effects of mental stress on myo-
believe that a placebo drug would have been equivalent to the
cardial ischemia during daily life. JAMA. 1997;277:1521–1526.
acquisition of a pet. Consequently, we cannot comment
5. Allen K, Blascovich J, Tomaka J, Kelsey RM. Presence of human friends
and pet dogs as moderators of autonomic responses to stress in women.
definitively on the possibility that the pet acted as a “placebo
J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61:582–589.
effect” or on the relationship between the power of sugges-
6. Christenfeld N, Gerin W, Linden W, Sanders M, Mathur J, Deich JD,
tion and the power of pets. Another limitation of the study is
Pickering TG. Social support effects on cardiovascular reactivity: is a
that because participants had stage II hypertension at the
stranger as effective as a friend? Psychosom Med. 1997;59:388 –398.
7. Fontana AM, Diegnan T, Villeneuve A, Lepore S. Nonevaluative social
beginning of the study, it was not ethically possible to
support reduces cardiovascular reactivity in young women during acutely
randomly assign only pet ownership for 6 months before
stressful performance situations. J Behav Med. 1999;22:75–91.
lisinopril therapy. This arm of the design would be especially
8. Kamarck TW, Manuck SB, Jennings JR. Social support reduces cardio-
vascular reactivity to psychological challenge: a laboratory model. Psy-
important in future investigations among populations with
borderline hypertension, because it would help determine
9. Lepore SJ, Allen KA, Evans GW. Social support lowers cardiovascular
whether manipulation of the social environment can reduce
reactivity to an acute stressor. Psychosom Med. 1993;55:518 –524.
the need for drug therapy. Yet another drawback to our study
10. Cohen S, Syme SL. Social Support and Health. San Diego, Calif:
is that although our findings reveal much about blood
11. Shumaker SA, Czajkowski SM. Social Support and Cardiovascular
pressure response to acute psychological stress in the home,
Disease. New York, NY: Plenum; 1994. 820 Hypertension October 2001
12. Friedmann E, Katcher AH, Lynch JJ, Thomas SA. Animal companions
19. Cohen S, Hoberman HM. Positive events and social supports as buffers of
and one-year survival of patients after discharge from a coronary care
life change to stress. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1983;13:99 –125.
unit. Public Health Rep. 1980;95:307–312.
20. O’Brien AA, Bulpitt CJ. The effects of ACE inhibitors on cognitive
13. Friedmann E, Thomas SA. Pet ownership, social support, and one-year
function. Drugs Aging. 1995;6:173–180.
survival after acute myocardial infarction in the Cardiac Arrhythmia
21. Delbecq AL, Van de Ven AH, Gustafson DH. Group Techniques for
Suppression Trial (CAST). Am J Cardiol. 1995;76:1213–1217. Program Planning. Middleton, Wis: Green Briar Press; 1986.
14. Siegel JM. Stressful life events and use of physician services among the
22. Waeber B, Brunner HR. Clinical value of ambulatory blood pressure
elderly: the moderating role of pet ownership. J Pers Soc Psychol.
monitoring in the assessment of antihypertensive therapy. Blood Press
15. Siegel JM, Angulo FJ, Detels R, Wesch J, Mullen A. AIDS diagnosis and
23. Dimsdale JE, Ziegler JB, Mills P. Renin correlates with blood pressure
depression in the multicenter AIDS cohort study: the ameliorating impact
reactivity to stressors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1990;3:237–242.
of pet ownership. AIDS Care. 1999;11:157–169.
16. Allen K, Blascovich J. The value of service dogs for people with severe
24. Spence JD, Manuck SB, Munoz C, Cheung H, Huff M, Dennis B,
ambulatory disabilities: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 1996;275:
Borkowski K. Hemodynamic and endocrine effects of mental stress in
untreated borderline hypertension. Am J Hypertens. 1990;3:859 – 862.
17. Campbell DT, Stanley JC. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental
25. Case DB, Wallace JM, Keim HJ, Weber MA, Sealey JE, Laragh JH. Designs for Research. Boston, Mass: Houghton Mifflin Company; 1963.
Possible role of renin in hypertension as suggested by renin-sodium
18. Kelsey RM. Electrodermal lability and myocardial reactivity to stress.
profiling and inhibition of converting enzyme. N Engl J Med. 1977;296:
Psychophysiology. 1991;28:619 – 632.
PRESCRIVERE IL TAI CHI PER LA FIBROMIALGIA- CI RISIAMO? Gloria Y.Yeh, M.D., M.P.H., Ted J. Kaptchuck and Robert H Shmerling, M.D. La fibromialgia è una malattia dolorosa comune e poco conosciuta che colpisce 200 milioni di persone e più in tutto il mondo. L'assenza di alterazioni oggettive all'esame fisico nonchè alla diagnostica di laboratorio e per immagini ha condotto molti medici a dub
SAFETY DATA SHEET according to EC directive 2001/58/EC Basacote LR 12M (15+8+13S+2+TE) 1. IDENTIFICATION OF THE SUBSTANCE/PREPARATION AND OF THE COMPANY/UNDERTAKING Product information 2. COMPOSITION/INFORMATION ON INGREDIENTS Chemical nature NPK - fertilizer containing: Ammonium Nitrate, ammonium salts, phosphates, salts of calcium, potassium and possibly magnesi